www射-国产免费一级-欧美福利-亚洲成人福利-成人一区在线观看-亚州成人

WORLD> America
Worms do calculus to find food
(Agencies)
Updated: 2008-07-28 17:45

Like humans with a nose for the best restaurants, roundworms also use their senses of taste and smell to navigate. And now, researchers may have found how a worm's brain does this: It performs calculus.

This file picture shows the track of a worm in search of food. The color purple represents the starting point. Red is the end point as well as the peak of the gradient. Researchers find that roundworms perform calculus to find food. [Agenceis]

Worms calculate how much the strength of different tastes is changing - equivalent to the process of taking a derivative in calculus - to figure out if they are on their way toward food or should change direction and look elsewhere, says University of Oregon biologist Shawn Lockery, who thinks humans and other animals do the same thing.

This research could one day benefit some of the more than 200,000 Americans who detect a foul smell or taste that is actually pleasant or have a weakened or depleted ability to appreciate the scent of a lilac or savor the flavor of a juicy burger.

Related readings:
 Want scientific immortality? Name a sea worm
 Online Valentine cards may contain Internet worm

"The more we know about how taste and smell function - not just at the level of primary sensory neurons, but downstream in the brain - the better prepared we will be to understand when the system is broken," Lockery says.

With the aid of salt and chili peppers, Lockery reached the calculating-worms conclusion by studying two anatomically identical neurons from the worm's brain that collectively regulate behavior. These two neurons function like "on" and "off" gates in a computer in response to changes in salt concentration levels. This dubiously delicious discovery, detailed in the July 3 issue of the journal Nature, hints at the method for smelling and tasting that is thought to be common among a wide variety of species, including humans.

Like human visual systems that respond to the presence and absence of light, Lockery and colleagues found that when the left neuron fires as salt concentrations increase, the roundworm continues crawling in the same direction. The right neuron responds when salt concentrations decrease, and the worm turns in search of a saltier location.

Lockery said this is similar to a game of hot-and-cold with a child. But there is one key difference: the worm doesn't need an observer to say if it's getting closer to or farther from the target - the worm calculates the change by itself.

Observing the worm responding to changes in concentration suggested an experiment to see if the worm's brain computes derivatives. The mathematical concept of a derivative indicates the rate at which something, such as salt concentration, changes at a given point in time and space. So Lockery tried to verify that these neurons recognize changes in salt concentration and then tell the worm where food is and where it is not.

To do so, he artificially activated each neuron with capsaicin, the spicy component in chili peppers, which worms naturally cannot detect. Worms with capsaicin applied to the left neuron crawled forward. When the worm's brain indicated that the current motion leads to increasing salt concentrations, it continues moving in its original direction. But when the worm's right neuron is activated by capsaicin, it is duped into thinking the salt levels are decreasing. So the worm changes direction, hoping to find salt elsewhere.

"We found a new way to do calculus with neurons," Lockery said.

Previous studies have identified "on" and "off" cells in the brains of other chemosensory animals such as fruit flies, cockroaches, frogs, lobsters and rats. Given the strong similarities between the olfactory regions of the brains in rats and other mammals, Lockery says that humans should also be included in this list. So his work suggests that this circuit may be a universal derivative for smelling and tasting.

In response to the lingering mystery of why worms go toward salts in search of food, Lockery offers an untested theory that the decaying carcasses of invertebrates, like snails and earthworms, provide a common source of bacteria. Since animals are very salty inside, he thinks there could be a link between salt and bacteria in the wild.

主站蜘蛛池模板: 日韩影院久久 | videos性欧美| 一道本久久 | 1204国产成人精品视频 | 香港经典a毛片免费观看看 香港经典a毛片免费观看爽爽影院 | 蘑菇午夜三级 | 国产成人精品午夜在线播放 | 欧美精品一区二区精品久久 | 美国大毛片 | 美国免费毛片 | 日韩欧美一区二区在线观看 | 狠狠色丁香婷婷综合久久来 | 久草免费在线视频观看 | 怡红院免费全部视频在线视频 | 亚洲网站在线观看 | 中文三级视频 | 国产一级片毛片 | 一区二区三区欧美 | 亚洲男人天堂网站 | 欧美人成a视频www | 精品一区二区三区免费观看 | 日本一区二区三区不卡视频中文字幕 | 美国一级免费毛片 | 欧美极品大肚孕妇孕交 | 萌白酱粉嫩jk福利视频在线观看 | 亚洲欧洲国产成人精品 | 日韩视频一区二区 | 欧美一区二区三区视频在线 | 国内精品亚洲 | 午夜在线亚洲男人午在线 | 午夜影院h | 欧美一级视频在线观看欧美 | 日韩字幕 | 久久久久亚洲精品一区二区三区 | 亚洲欧美日韩精品久久亚洲区色播 | 国产精品亚洲精品一区二区三区 | 国内91视频 | 草草影院永久在线观看 | 国产婷婷成人久久av免费高清 | 精品一久久香蕉国产线看播放 | 欧洲美女a视频一级毛片 |